Ethology and Reproduction of Captive Giant Pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca)

作者: Devra G. Kleiman

DOI: 10.1111/J.1439-0310.1983.TB02139.X

关键词:

摘要: and Summary The activity rhythm, reproductive cycle, maintenance, social behavior of a pair captive giant pandas were studied at the National Zoological Park, Washington, D.C. Results from this study available literature indicate that: 1) Giant are probably nocturnal, although they may be polycyclic in activity. 2) Males more active than females, especially locomotor scent-marking activity 3) Activity levels inversely correlated with age. 4) There is single annual estrous period one to three days between March June, preceded by conspicuous behavioral changes female consisting depressed appetite, increased restlessness scent-marking, vocalizing. Behavioral male similar synchronized cycle. 5) The major communicatory modes through vocalizations. A variety postures used deposit urine anogenital gland secretions throughout environment. Most call types show considerable variation grading; prominent during agonistic activities. Visual signals only poorly developed. Tactile contact frequent interactions might predicted, considering solitary nature species. However, significant tactile limited initial stages encounters, avoidance intolerance occurring thereafter. 6) Both panda's copulatory posture temporal patterning copulation (numerous short mounts brief ejaculatory intromission long inter-ejaculation interval) differ significantly sexual reported for other closely related carnivores. Observations suggest that prior compatibility essential successful behavior. 7) Comparative data on panda reproduction 140 day average gestation length (range: 122–163 days) includes delay, either implantation or early development. The neonatalimaternal weight ratio smallest among Eutherian mammals. 8) Three characteristics— (a) the occurrence weak estrus autumn some females (b) variability duration “gestation” and (c) survivorship young despite litter size — fossil evidence much wider previous geographic distribution, have had flexible opportunistic strategy their past evolutionary history, recently undergone K-selection, increasing dietary habitat specialization.

参考文章(31)
E. S. Dierenfeld, H. F. Hintz, J. B. Robertson, P. J. Van Soest, O. T. Oftedal, Utilization of Bamboo by the Giant Panda Journal of Nutrition. ,vol. 112, pp. 636- 641 ,(1982) , 10.1093/JN/112.4.636
DEVRA G. KLEIMAN, WILLIAM B. KARESH, PENN R. CHU, Behavioural changes associated with oestrus in the Giant panda International Zoo Yearbook. ,vol. 19, pp. 217- 224 ,(1979) , 10.1111/J.1748-1090.1979.TB00568.X
Erik K. Fritzell, Reproduction of raccoons (Procyon lotor) in North Dakota American Midland Naturalist. ,vol. 100, pp. 253- 256 ,(1978) , 10.2307/2424800
B. Tschanz, M. Meyer-Holzapfel, S. Bachmann, Das Informationssystem bei Braunbären1 Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. ,vol. 27, pp. 47- 72 ,(2010) , 10.1111/J.1439-0310.1970.TB01860.X
NORMAN C. MELUN, Giant Panda Exhibit at the National Zoological Park, Washington International Zoo Yearbook. ,vol. 15, pp. 263- 265 ,(1975) , 10.1111/J.1748-1090.1975.TB01420.X
D. G. KLEIMAN, J. SEIDENSTICKER, Pandas in the wild: the giant pandas of wolong. Science. ,vol. 228, pp. 875- 876 ,(1985) , 10.1126/SCIENCE.228.4701.875
MILES S. ROBERTS, Growth and development of mother‐reared Red pandas International Zoo Yearbook. ,vol. 15, pp. 57- 63 ,(1975) , 10.1111/J.1748-1090.1975.TB01354.X
Del Thiessen, Maureen Rice, Mammalian Scent Gland Marking and Social Behavior Psychological Bulletin. ,vol. 83, pp. 505- 539 ,(1976) , 10.1037/0033-2909.83.4.505
VINCENT M. SARICH, The Giant Panda is a Bear Nature. ,vol. 245, pp. 218- 220 ,(1973) , 10.1038/245218A0