The Detection and the Neural Correlates of Behavioral (Prior) Intentions

作者: Sara Agosta , Umberto Castiello , Davide Rigoni , Stefano Lionetti , Giuseppe Sartori

DOI: 10.1162/JOCN_A_00039

关键词:

摘要: Prior intentions are mental representations that are believed to be the prime cause of our intentional actions. To date, only a few studies have focused on the possibility that single prior intentions could be identified in people's minds. Here, for the first time, we used the autobiographical Implicit Association Test (aIAT) in order to identify a specific prior intention on the basis of a pattern of associations derived from reaction times (Experiment 1). The aIAT is based on two critical blocks: the block associating intentions with true sentences (congruent block) gave rise to faster reaction times (RTs) than the block associating intentions with false sentences (incongruent block). Furthermore, when comparing intentions with hopes, it was revealed that the reported effect was intention-specific: The pattern of associations reflected a congruency effect when intentions and the logical category “True” were paired, but not when hopes and the “True” category were paired (Experiment 2). Finally, we investigated the neural bases of the congruency effect that leads to the identification of an intention (Experiment 3). We found a reduced late positive component (LPC) for the incongruent with respect to the congruent block, suggesting that the incongruent block needs additional resources of cognitive control with respect to the congruent block.

参考文章(48)
Cristina Becchio, Luisa Sartori, Maria Bulgheroni, Umberto Castiello, The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: A kinematic study on social intention Consciousness and Cognition. ,vol. 17, pp. 557- 564 ,(2008) , 10.1016/J.CONCOG.2007.03.003
Ray Johnson, Heather Henkell, Elizabeth Simon, John Zhu, The self in conflict: The role of executive processes during truthful and deceptive responses about attitudes NeuroImage. ,vol. 39, pp. 469- 482 ,(2008) , 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2007.08.032
M Kutas, S. Hillyard, Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity Science. ,vol. 207, pp. 203- 205 ,(1980) , 10.1126/SCIENCE.7350657
Peter M. Gollwitzer, Goal Achievement: The Role of Intentions European Review of Social Psychology. ,vol. 4, pp. 141- 185 ,(1993) , 10.1080/14792779343000059
Jack B. Isreal, Gregory L. Chesney, Christopher D. Wickens, Emanuel Donchin, P300 and Tracking Difficulty: Evidence For Multiple Resources in Dual‐Task Performance Psychophysiology. ,vol. 17, pp. 259- 273 ,(1980) , 10.1111/J.1469-8986.1980.TB00146.X
Chun Siong Soon, Marcel Brass, Hans-Jochen Heinze, John-Dylan Haynes, Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain Nature Neuroscience. ,vol. 11, pp. 543- 545 ,(2008) , 10.1038/NN.2112
CYNTHIA DOUCET, ROBERT M. STELMACK, The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time. Psychophysiology. ,vol. 36, pp. 351- 363 ,(1999) , 10.1017/S0048577299980563
Icek Ajzen, Martin Fishbein, Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research Philosophy and Rhetoric. ,vol. 10, ,(1975)
Gilles O. Einstein, Melissa J. Guynn, Mark A. McDaniel, Sarah L. Richardson, Allison R. Cunfer, Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. ,vol. 21, pp. 996- 1007 ,(1995) , 10.1037//0278-7393.21.4.996
Patrick Haggard, Conscious intention and motor cognition Trends in Cognitive Sciences. ,vol. 9, pp. 290- 295 ,(2005) , 10.1016/J.TICS.2005.04.012