Predation risk and herd position influence the proportional use of antipredator and social vigilance by impala

作者: Anita van Deventer , Adrian M. Shrader

DOI: 10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2020.11.019

关键词:

摘要: Vigilance is just one of the many fitness-enhancing activities that animals do each day. However, as vigilance used for both predator detection and obtaining social information, individuals must decide how to divide their time between these two types. Yet, it unclear (1) prey species living in centre edge groups prioritize use different types (2) this prioritization varies with increasing risk. To explore this, we focused on degree which impala, Aepyceros melampus, a herd-living antelope, adjusted antipredator (looking out from herd) at herd members) across three sites guilds densities. We found densities increased, size but impala did not adjust total they spent vigilant (antipredator plus vigilance). Thus, sacrifice other (e.g. foraging) increase vigilance. overall, within herds, displayed greater social) showed proportional compared central individuals. With numbers, maintained ultimately increased In contrast, reduced As suggested elsewhere, adjustment might have been related trying foraging efficiency cohesion benefits. may also monitored conspecifics detect threats, while short- less vulnerable head-down position) long-term daily allowing more activities) Ultimately, our results highlight position differences can influence overall levels

参考文章(75)
Steven L. Lima, Peter A. Bednekoff, Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack? Animal Behaviour. ,vol. 58, pp. 537- 543 ,(1999) , 10.1006/ANBE.1999.1182
Eben Goodale, Guy Beauchamp, Robert D. Magrath, James C. Nieh, Graeme D. Ruxton, Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure Trends in Ecology and Evolution. ,vol. 25, pp. 354- 361 ,(2010) , 10.1016/J.TREE.2010.01.002
Fred S. Guthery, Kenneth P. Burnham, David R. Anderson, Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach The Journal of Wildlife Management. ,vol. 67, pp. 655- ,(2003) , 10.2307/3802723
Ron Ritter, Peter A. Bednekoff, VIGILANCE IN NXAI PAN SPRINGBOK, ANTIDORCAS MARSUPIALIS Behaviour. ,vol. 129, pp. 1- 11 ,(1994) , 10.1163/156853994X00325
Yamil Di Blanco, Ben T. Hirsch, Determinants of vigilance behavior in the ring-tailed coati ( Nasua nasua ): the importance of within-group spatial position Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. ,vol. 61, pp. 173- 182 ,(2006) , 10.1007/S00265-006-0248-3
Steven V. Viscido, David S. Wethey, Quantitative analysis of fiddler crab flock movement: evidence for ‘selfish herd’ behaviour Animal Behaviour. ,vol. 63, pp. 735- 741 ,(2002) , 10.1006/ANBE.2001.1935
M. Valeix, A. J. Loveridge, S. Chamaillé-Jammes, Z. Davidson, F. Murindagomo, H. Fritz, D. W. Macdonald, Behavioral adjustments of African herbivores to predation risk by lions: Spatiotemporal variations influence habitat use Ecology. ,vol. 90, pp. 23- 30 ,(2009) , 10.1890/08-0606.1
Craig Packer, Peter Abrams, Should co-operative groups be more vigilant than selfish groups? Journal of Theoretical Biology. ,vol. 142, pp. 341- 357 ,(1990) , 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80557-0
C. S. Holling, The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Memoirs of the Entomological Society of Canada. ,vol. 97, pp. 5- 60 ,(1965) , 10.4039/ENTM9745FV